Thursday, 4 July 2019

The 4-stage approach to teaching a skill in ATLS: A review of literature



Context

The 4 – and subsequently 5 step model for teaching a skill in ATLS has been around since the ATLS programme began in 1978, and has remained one of the most controversial areas of the now global programme. This paper reviews the history and development of the model, provides previously unacknowledged evidence for its use and proposes ways in which we can better communicate and use the model in our ATLS courses.

Introduction

Time for skill acquisition is severely limited on ATLS courses, and always has been, with priority having been given pre 10th edition to the dissemination of the knowledge-based areas of the course manual through lectures. The 2019 10th edition provides a greater emphasis on skills learning through scenario-based discussions, thereby maximising the context of psychomotor skills within simulated practice rather than as an adjunct to the manual chapters. Despite this pedagogical advancement, time is still restricted and instructors may still be privileging lower levels of Bloom’s cognitive taxonomy over the acquisition of higher order evaluative skills and psychomotor competence development. It is acknowledged that the ATLS Provider (student) course is a two-day course with at least three days’ worth of learning crammed into the time available. Current challenges include how much pre course learning can be expected for psychomotor skills and how best to maximise the time on course we do have to practise.

Rationale

This paper will:

·        - Review the nature of and need for psychomotor skill acquisition in the ATLS context;
 - Clarify the psychological processes involved in acquiring a new skill;
 - Reflect on a range of relevant literature;
 - Offer hitherto unacknowledged evidence for the 4-stage method; 

Psychomotor skills– what are they?

Psychomotor skills are described as “the mental and motor activities required to execute a manual task,” according to Foley & Spilansky (1972) in Kovacs (1997) and a concept of “integration of well-adjusted performances, rather than a tying together of mere habits” (Adams, 1987). “Skills are both mental and motor and they are learned,” asserts Adams (ibid). He adds, “any behaviour that has been called skilled involves combinations of cognitive, perceptual and motor processes with different weights” (p.42).

Singer adds to this: “Skilled motor activity is a function of Input (sensory and perceptual function) x Central processing (decision and command functions) x Output (motor functions). The integration of these processes leads to more purposeful behaviour” (Singer, 1978)

To summarise, psychomotor skills, are behaviours that require a combination of cognitive, sensory, perceptual, organisational and motor processes, acquired by novices and practised using all of the above, until they become proficient (Dreyfus & Dreyfus 1986.)

Psychomotor skills – previous research.

There is a dearth of evidence in this field pertaining to the medical field. Procedural medicine has been an “assumed activity...without a formal educational context, living within the cliché: ‘see one, do one, teach one’ for too long” says Kovacs ((1996, p387). This may be due to the preference for randomised control trials over other approaches which have yielded better results in the psychological material. So, we tend not to find studies on experts and novices in skills acquisition, and there is very little work on how we teach and learn psychomotor skills. But that does not mean that because there is no model, and no RCT to support it, we cannot support the approach we have been using for 40 years in ATLS from other, related fields. The domains of sports and physical education have some useful work we can draw on and may well offer a starting point for the much-needed empirical research we need in this area of medicine.

Three psychomotor domains were created during the 1970s – Simpson (1972), Harrow (1972) and Dave (1967). Simpson’s model was the one used by the originators of ATLS, with some adaptation. All three models aimed to describe what happened when people performed a skill, and did not focus on the process to acquire the skill or to transfer a skill from a master to a novice. In short, the 4-stage approach was developed by George and Doto (ibid) as outlined later in their 2001 paper in Family Medicine, by extrapolation from Simpson’s work, but there is little background evidence of what contributed to that model. More recent writers have adopted George and Doto’s original ATLS 4 stage model (1991) model, even being erroneously credited for it (Peyton, 1998). Before dismissing the model, should we not find evidence for its use, and if that is not possible within the medical community, bound as it is by narrow research preferences, then in other areas of research?

What do we know about learning psychomotor skills?

Some of the original work in this area is decades old, and to be found outside of the medical education literature, but with little evidence given for the ATLS 4/5 stage model, this work is offered to clarify how and why the 4/5 stage model has been advocated for so long.

1.Singer and Cauraugh (1985) identify the three elements of learning a skill:

·        acquisition,
·        retention and
·        transfer potential (p104.)

In identifying a model for the acquisition of psychomotor skills in ATLS I suggest we select a model that enables our students to acquire, retain and transfer the skill to their practice. If any of these factors are omitted, the skill will not have been acquired for future use, it will have merely been replicated from the short-term memory. When we debate a model for skill acquisition, we need to be mindful that acquisition is only the first step, and that without retention and transfer the skill has not been learned and cannot thereby be used safely.

Summary: Skills need to be acquired, retained and transferred appropriate to patient need. We need a model that enables this over mere momentary imitation.


2.Barnes (1987) emphasises two principles in the teaching and learning of psychomotor skills:

i)           To prevent learners developing faulty initial habits which are then very difficult to unlearn
ii)          Skill retention correlates with the level of initial proficiency – thus requiring that the first time a learner practises a skill it has to be practised accurately.

A similar focus on initial accuracy followed by later practice for speed is outlined by Smith et al (1997) in their work on learning to perform fibre optic nasotracheal intubation.

Summary: Skills need to be performed correctly the first time; if error is permitted retention will be delayed, if achieved at all. We need a model that best enables correct practice first time round.

3.To achieve the three elements in (1) and the two principles in (2), Singer and Cauraugh (ibid) categorise learning and performance strategies as primary and secondary. Primary strategies relate to conducting the skill, (also called associative by Gagne & Briggs, 1974.) Secondary strategies allow the primary strategies to operate more effectively – also called support (Dansereau,1978). A primary strategy may result in little more than copying, whereas a secondary strategy leads to better retention, adaptation to other situations and self-management. I suggest our role as instructors is to equip learners with the secondary strategies so that they can practise ATLS safely without our presence. Models to enable this are discussed subsequently.

Summary: the mechanism of acquiring a skill needs to sustain its practitioner once alone

4.Barnes (1987) who ironically characterises surgery as the “ultimate body contact sport,” pointed to Lippert and Farmer (1984) in reminding us that psychomotor development has to take place alongside cognitive and affective development. Indeed Simpson (1966, 1972), the originator of the work used by the 4 stage approach creators, George and Doto (1991) also characterised behaviour in the psychomotor domain as reflexive of the mental, emotional and physical states of the performer.

Singer & Cauraugh (ibid) acknowledge that: “a great deal of information processing occurs…..when people attempt to learn/perform complex motor activities” (p106) and that one of the “primary differences between the highly skilled and the lesser skilled is the degree and type of conscious involvement prior to, during and following motor performance” (Singer, Garson & Kim, 1979).

Summary: Skills acquisition is cognitive, not just motor. We need a model that allows for cognitive acquisition before, during and alongside the motor acquisition.

5.We need to be aware of the dangers of adapting only an instructional strategy whereby a student acquires a skill quickly, but with no agency over it. Rather we should be using a self-initiated strategy
Whereby the student not only acquires the skill, but develops a self-generating strategy for its future use. (Singer and Cauraugh, ibid, p107)

Summary: Developing a schema that allows future use of the skill is essential

Evidence for the 4-stage model for skills teaching as used in ATLS since 1978.

This is by no means an exhaustive review of the evidence for the 4/5 stage approach. What this selection offers is a perspective on skills acquisition that is to be found outside of the medical field. The domains of physical education and cognitive psychology have been sampled for this purpose.

The following components are considered vital to any effective psychomotor skills teaching and learning approach:

The role of context

“Performance in a discrete, procedural task is forgotten more readily than performance in a continuous task” (Adams & Hufford, 1962; Ammons et al, 1958; Mengelkoch et al, 1971; Neumann & Ammons, 1975).


The role of cognition

The role of cognitive processes has perhaps been ignored by detractors of the 4/5 stage approach. For clarity, “a cognitive process is a control process that is a self-generated, transient, situationally determined, conscious activity a learner uses to organize and regulate received and transmitted information, and ultimately behaviour” (Singer, 1980a). We cannot ignore that people actively seek to control their environments, and are not passive recipients of situational dictates. We need to recognise and utilise this in the process of transferring a skill from an expert to a novice. That means that the learner needs to be an active participant, drawing on their cognitive processes, with support and guidance from their instructor. Learning a psychomotor skill is a cognitive strategy which draws on internal organisation as well as external instruction, both of which govern the learner’s behaviour. A learner imposes a structure on their movement information that ensures it is learned and retrieved more efficiently.

The role of visual input

Vision is the dominant modality in the early stages of learning (Fleishman & Rich, 1963) but gives way to other sensory modalities later on in the learning process.

The role of verbal description

The development of those organisational pathways outlined above can be mediated by the external support of the instructor. This support is enacted largely through language, or verbalisation.

“the ability to succeed in motor performance may very well be related to the ability to apply external and internal words to motor acts” says Singer in O’Neil (1978).

Not only are words spoken by the instructor important but also the words spoken by the learner to guide themselves.  This has been variously termed: verbal mediation, conceptualisation, ideation and thought processing.

There is a long history of recognition of the importance of verbal support for motor skill acquisition. In 1953 McAllister found that verbal learning transferred positively to motor learning, and verbal pre- training transferred to motor responses in visual-motor tasks. In 1955 Goss acknowledged that “verbal mediators were seen as implicit agents contributing to movement regulation.”  However verbal pre-training is a transfer theory, and is a highly empirical domain, not in favour with medical education researchers. Whilst interest in mediation and pre training theory died in the late 1960s and early 1970s, sadly it does not mean that a better theory for the verbal regulation of movement has emerged since (Zivin, 1979). We would benefit form some empirical research in this area.

The need for closed loop learning (Adams, 1971)

A closed loop system has feedback from the response, error detection and error correction.  Central to closed loop theory is the student acquisition of capacity to detect and correct errors, and the growth in this process is central to the learning process.  It rejects Thorndike’s behavioural and unconscious view of learning which posits that skills are merely habits to be acquired without agent consciousness. The axiom underpinning this is that motor learning is at heart a perceptual process. The use of error information and the feedback -perceptual trace comparison is a verbal and cognitive activity. Feedback ‘in-situ’ is vital to this process.


The role of Feedback in-situ

Feedback (auditory, proprioceptive and visual) is an integral part of motor control. The identification and correction of motor errors is highly dependent on it (Keele, 1982.) Closed loop learning without feedback in the moment, is not effective. Error detection and auto correction is essential to closed loop learning. Furthermore, if we use secondary strategies within the closed loop learning system, we show that an internal reference for judgment of error develops. Students become over time, their own self corrector.

Kovacs (1997) acknowledges that “although ‘correction and reinforcement’ is listed as a separate stage, in his work, and is often seen as the fifth stage in the ATLS model,  it should be incorporated into each of the preceding steps because it provides a form of feedback for knowledge of results.” (p390).  We consider the role of in-situ feedback to be an inherent and essential part of stages 3 and 4, and not a separate stage.

Early evidence of the 4-stage approach

    1)   Hall 1978
Perhaps the earliest evidence of the 4-stage approach can be found in Hall’s work (1978) which suggests students need to:
     

  •    Think about or imagine motor responses or movement patterns in particular situations.
  •      Attach verbal labels to each of a series of movements
  •      Selectively attend to relevant components of the task
  •      Verbally rehearse a sequence


   2)     Singer & Suwanthada, 1984
This model was intended for self-paced skills – those which are proactive, not reactive:

  •      Readying – obtaining an optimal mental – emotional state for task and situation
  •      Imaging – the role of self talk
  •      Focusing – excluding extraneous information
  •      Executing – using the self talk to guide practice and focus.


   3)      Kovacs (1997) 

Kovacs acknowledges the need to divide the skills acquisition process into two stages:

(i)         A cognitive one whereby the learner conceptualises the “broader context of the skills” (p390) which relate to both the visual stage 1 of George and Doto’s (1991) model and the verbal stage 2.
(ii)        A more active, motor stage whereby learners both verbalise and perform the procedure from start to finish, which relate to stages 3 and 4.

It is not difficult to see where the 4-stage approach to teaching a skill had its roots. 

There is a dearth of evidence in the medical literature or the medical education literature, but that does not mean that we should not look to other areas of professional expertise for clarification. This review is only the beginning of a process that is long overdue.



References
Adams, JA (1971) A Closed-Loop Theory of Motor Learning.  Journal of Motor Behaviour 3. 111 - 149
Adams, JA (1987) Historical review and appraisal of research on the learning retention and transfer of human motor skills. Psychological Bulletin 101. 41-74
Adams, JA & Hufford, LE (1962) Contributions of a part-task trainer to the learning and re-learning of a time-shared flight manoeuvre, Human Factors 4. 159-170
Ammons, RB., Farr, RG., Bloch, E., Neumann, E., Dey, M., Marion, R., Ammons, CH., (1958) Long term retention of perceptual motor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology 55. 318 - 328
Barnes, RW (1987) Surgical Handicraft – teaching and learning surgical skills. American Journal of Surgery. 153 (5) 422 - 427
Bloom, B (1977) Taxonomy of Educational Objective: handbook 1: Cognitive Domain. New York. Lagman Publishers.
Dansereau, DF (1978) The Development of a Learning Strategies Curriculum in O’Neil, HF (Ed) Learning Strategies, New York. Academic Press
Dave, R. H. (1967). Psychomotor domain. Presented at the International Conference of Educational Testing, Berlin.
Dreyfus, SE (1986) The Five-Stage Model of Adult Skill Acquisition. Bulletin of Science Technology & Society 2004 24: 177
Fleishman, ES., Rich, D (1963) Role of Kinaesthetic and Spatial-Visual Abilities in Perceptual Motor Learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology 66. 6-11
Foley, RP & Spilansky, J (1972) Teaching Techniques – a handbook for health professionals. New York. McGraw Hill 1980 71-91
George & Doto (1991) A Simple Five Step Method for Teaching Clinical Skills. Family Medicine  33(8):577-8 · October 2001
Goss, AE (1955) Stimulus Response Analysis of the Interaction of Cue Producing in Instrumental Responses. Psychological Review 62. 20-31
Hall, CR (1978) A Review of Encoding Processes in Verbal and motor Memory. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sceinces
Harrow, AJ (1972) A Taxonomy of the Psychomotor Domain. New York. David McKay
Keele, SW (1982) Component Analysis and Conceptions of Skill in Kelso, JAS (Ed) Human Motor Beahviour: an introduction. Hilsdale, New jersey. Erlbaum
Kovacs, G (1971) Procedural skills in medicine: linking theory to practice. Journal of Emergency Medicine 15 (3) 387 - 391
Lippert, FGI and Farmer, JA (1984) Psychomotor Skills in Orthopaedic Surgery. Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore
McAllister, DE (1953) The Effects of Various Kinds of Relevant Verbal pre Training on Subsequent motor Performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology 46. 329 - 336
Mengelkoch, RF., Adams, JA., Gainer, CA (1971) The Forgetting of Instrument flying Skills. Human Factors 13, 379 – 405
Neumann, E., Ammons, RB., (1957) Acquisition and Long term Retention of a Simple Serial perceptual Motor Skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 53. 159 – 161
O’Neil, HF (Ed) (1978) Learning Strategies. New York. Academic Press
Peyton JW. Teaching and Learning in Medical Practice. Heronsgate Rickmansworth, Herts.: Manticore Europe Ltd; 1998.
Simpson, EJ (1972) The Classification of Educational Objectives in the Psychomotor Domain in National Specific Media institute (ed) The Psychomotor Domain: A resource book for media specialists. Washington DC. Gryphon House
Singer, RN., (1978) Psychomotor domain in O’Neil, HF (1978) Learning Strategies, Academic Press Inc., London, UK.
Singer, RN., Garson, RF., Kim, K (1979) Information Processing Capabilities in performers Differing in Levels of Motor Skills. Tech. Rep. Tr-79-A4, Motor Behaviour Resources Centre, Florida State University, Tallahassee
Singer, RN., Stelmach, GE., Requin, J (eds)(1980) Motor Beahviour and the Role of Cognitive Processes and Learners Strategies in Tutorials in Motor Beahviour. Amsterdam, Netherlands
Singer, RN., Cauraugh (1985) The Generalizability Effect of Learning Strategies for categories of Psychomotor Skills. QUEST. 37. 103 - 119
Smith. JE., Jackson, APP., Hurdely, J., Clifton, PJM (1997) Learning Curves for Fibreoptics nasotracheal intubation when using endoscopic video camera. Anaesthesia 52 (2) 101-106
Zivin, G (Ed) (1979) The Development of Self Regulation through Speech. New York. Wiley

No comments:

Post a Comment