Sunday 29 May 2011

Pendleton's rules

At the recent ATLS National Day and the Europen meeting of ATLS Educators there were discussions about other courses trying out different approaches to feedback. It seems as though Pendleton's rules for feedback http://www.gp-training.net/training/educational_theory/feedback/pendleton.htm are falling out of favour.

I was asked why this may be the case, and my personal view is that it is not the Pendleton framework which is at fault, but the struggle many people have to either understand the point of Pendleton, or to conduct a developmental conversation that is both specific, based on behavioural evidence and sufficiently constructive.

I have discussed various forms of feedback on here previously (see DEBRIEF), but wanted to make out a case for the use of Pendleton's rules.

For me the benefit of Pendleton is that it reaches all stages of learning, from the competent to the incompetent, and from that which we are aware of to that which we are not aware of.

As I have written before, learning is a matter of developing both competence and conscious awareness. We progress from a state of not knowing that we do not know or cannot do (unconscious incompetence) through the stage of being aware of what we do not know or cannot do (conscious incompetence) to one of knowing what we know and can do (conscious competence) to the final stage of knowing what we know and doing what we do but not being always aware of that (unconscious competence.)

If this is the case, and this learning curve describes the rudimentary stages we progress through when learning a new skill or behaviour, then feedback needs to access each of these stages of learning.

Pendleton's rules map onto these stages of learning beautifully:

1. Asking "What went well with that?" accesses the conscious competence quadrant and focuses the learner's mind on practice that needs to be repeated in future. This question also allows for the teacher to assess the levels of insight the learner displays, in their self evaluation.

2. Providing further discussion of what went well led by the teacher, develops the good practice and may access the areas of strength which the learner has no awareness of (unconsciously competent.)

3. Asking the learner what they were less pleased with and what could be developed further, also checks insight levels, and accesses the consciously incompetent areas of practice.

4. Finally, discussing with the learner what the teacher feels needs to be developed (with an action plan to do so) accesses the unconsciously incompetent quadrant.

Pendleton offers a framework within which we can discuss all aspects of the learning curve, including those areas of competence and incompetence known and unknown to the learner. What we need to do within each of the four questions advocated by Pendleton is to be specific about the strengths and areas for development, not shy away from being honest in our descriptions of the behaviours we have observed. And we must always encourage further actions which develop the weaker areas.

Monday 16 May 2011

Proof it can be done!

A great new article from a trainee who was victim to the curriculum changes from BST to ISCP.

This article shows how hard Rafay worked and what he has achieved. He is now deservedly in ST3 training in the specialty of his choice.

http://careers.bmj.com/careers/advice/view-article.html?id=20002902

Thursday 13 January 2011

DEBRIEF: A reflective tool for workplace based learning
Hayley Allan

Why do we sometimes act inconsistently? Our heart is pulling us in one direction and our head is insisting on another. What about that gut feeling we have? You know that niggling feeling in the middle of the night? The knowledge, deep down somewhere that what we are doing is not totally right?

Survival has always depended on gut feeling. Humans would long be extinct if they did not have the ability to instinctively know when something is wrong. How many times do we hear someone saying that they “just knew” something was wrong? Paediatricians know that if the mother is worried, they ought to be worried. The girl who was mugged outside the internal front door of her second floor flat, knew there was something not quite right.  What is this sixth sense we all have and why are we advised to ignore it at our peril?

Call it intuition, call it experience. (It will depend on where you were schooled. If in the arts or social sciences you may favour intuition as a term for this phenomenon. Medics and those who deal with the allegedly more concrete world would call it experience. )

Experience is a tremendous learning tool because it develops in us over time; it steadily drip feeds our psyche while we work. Experience builds up pattern recognition over time. It swells the coffers of our intellect, adding to the vault of events and feelings that our mind stockpiles over the years. It is a rich resource. Many people, especially those who learned their craft through trial and error, through experimentation and throughput of events, believe there is no other way to learn.

But what if that accumulation of experience could be fast tracked? What if, instead of laying down lots of fifty pence pieces in the bank vault (individually heavy and of low value) we put by the (admittedly less frequent) stash of ten pound notes we came across? Is there a way to take more learning from fewer learning experiences?

Learning from experience and developing metacognition

DEBRIEF is a tool that enables reflection to take place between a number of people or individually. It provides a structure for review of an event in an emotional, a cognitive and a practical way, thus addressing the psychosocial and practical elements of learning. It has been acknowledged that learning is facilitated or hampered by emotions (Boekaerts 1993, Goleman 1995) and that emotions drive learning and memory (Sylvester 1994.) Learning is not a purely cognitive process (Le Doux (1997) Gross (2008) Love & Goodsell (1996). Much has been said about the emotional impact of learning. If we agree that learning is based on experiences then we cannot deny that emotions will play a part in those experiences and how we process them. The DEBRIEF model, in the constructivist tradition, helps learners to “take responsibility for their own learning, to be autonomous thinkers, to develop integrated understandings of concepts and to pose – and seek to answer- important questions.” Brooks & Brooks (1993)

Friere (1970) argued that learning and education is transformed through praxis – that is “reflection and action upon the world in order to transform it.” Vygotsky (1978 ) identified a zone of proximal development – a gap in terms of experience between two people at different levels of performance which could be used to “scaffold” (Wood et al 1976) the learning for the less experienced of the two. Scaffolding works best when functioning in a situated context or a Community of Practice (Lave & Wenger 1991) and remains the most practically useful way for many in training to learn. DEBRIEF offers a simple structure for such scaffolding to follow, but can also develop in time into internal DEBRIEFing, or metacognition.

Where are the trainers? And how do I know what I don’t know?

In current workplaces, trainees rarely have a supervisor with them all the time; the luxury of a more expert person always being at the elbow of the novice to question and support them in their thinking and practice is but a dream. If medical training is to be ‘trainee driven’ ( ISCP 2010, RCPath 2011,RCP 2011 ) then learners have to recognise their own learning needs and seek out an expert with whom to discuss those needs. However many needs, or gaps in knowledge and uncertainty about practice, fall into the zone of ‘unconscious incompetence.’ How can we know what we do not know if we do not know it and do not know that we do not know it? Once we are consciously competent (that is we know what we don’t know) there is not a problem, but often this conscious awareness has to be raised by either experience or a supervisor. If the supervisors aren’t there, we return to the learning by mistakes method, which is no longer tenable in the twenty first century.

This is where gut feeling comes in.


Fig 1. The role of conscious awareness in the development of competence.

There is a halfway house between unconscious incompetence and conscious incompetence where gut feeling resides. It is a small space, barely perceptible to some, but it can be developed given practice and the right conditions. Gut feeling can alert the learner to an inconsistency, or a ‘perturbation’ (Piaget 1954, 1971) and it is then the role of the learner to pursue this. There are several ways to do this, and usually the educational response is to seek out a supervisor or mentor for a conversation such as a Case Based Discussion, or to reflect on the perturbation independently. Both approaches can have limited effect. A CBD may yield a sophisticated level of analysis, resulting in new levels of understanding for trainee and even for the trainer. Often however, the conversation becomes didactic and theoretical and the synthesis between knowledge and application can be lost. Independent reflection is a good habit to develop but if the gut feeling is not explored purposefully and systematically there will be no real development beyond the ever decreasing circles we can be trapped in when trying to work out ‘what went wrong.’

DEBRIEFing

Debriefing is a mixture of reflecting and teaching. Using a framework to discuss the gut feeling with a more experienced colleague can lead to unexpected revelations. Once the framework has been practised several times it can work without another person’s input. If the learner becomes accustomed to following the steps in the model, s/he can uncover information and understanding to which they did not have conscious access previously either with a peer or alone.


DEBRIEF model



Describe events as factually as possible

Evaluate what went well/to change next time

Bring out emotions/values/beliefs/assumptions that cloud judgement and development

Review and analyse in light of previous experience; what a colleague would have done

Identify lessons learned

Establish follow up actions

Feedback on actions


©Hayley Allan 2009


Fig 2 DEBRIEF model
 
How does DEBRIEF work?

DEBRIEF is more than reflection. It is a series of questions asked of the learner which promote recall of the events, evaluation of his or her role in the events, and a psychological review of the impact of the events on the learner’s sense of wellbeing, before addressing the cognitive impact and reviewing the account for previous similarities of behaviour. Often perturbations (Piaget 1954, 1971) occur because we repeat behaviour which is a function of emotional or psychological triggers from past experiences. It is only when learners can look back in a safe environment, knowing that whilst they may have made mistakes they also had a positive effect on some of the events concerning them, that they are free to own those mistakes. Emotions can block cognitive development and progress and so the examining of the feelings, beliefs, assumptions or motives behind the learner’s actions is often a pivotal point at which the individual unblocks that repetitive behaviour or identifies the gut feeling causing the perturbation. Being able to move onto action planning as a result of the identification of what has been learned is a positive and valuable way for the learner to move on from the event.

How does DEBRIEF differ from regular reflection?

Reflection often follows the Learning cycle of Kolb (1984) but rarely bridges the gap between the action and reflection stages or between reflection and theory stages. Most learners are not able to make those large transitions alone without additional structure.



Fig 3 Kolb’s reflective practice cycle




Fig 4 Kolb’s cycle with DEBRIEF added

DEBRIEF provides a step wise structure to enable the learner to progress through each of Kolb’s learning points, but looking in turn at the behavioural, psychological and cognitive elements of the practice. By following these steps when reviewing an experience the learner is not only processing the experience itself, but is also developing metacognition which will enable further reflection on future experiences.

Building on Pendleton

Many trainers use Pendleton’s rules in discussing performance with trainees. Pendleton’s central tenets are learner comments preceding trainer’s comments, and positive features preceding developmental areas. This is included in the second step of the DEBRIEF model in order to review the actions taken and behaviour used. Without the emotional and cognitive areas of the experience being looked at too, the feedback can remain formulaic and focused on the surface actions rather than the motives or thinking underlying them.

The impact of DEBRIEF on learning

DEBRIEFing makes learners feel in control of their work; by instigating the process and by learning from an honest DEBRIEF, a trainee can relate to the strengths and areas for development within their practice, can understand the psychological impact of events and is able to access theoretical, emotional and practical developmental strategies to change that work for the better. Ownership of learning, especially in the workplace is a fundamental driver for progress. DEBRIEFing using this structure is a versatile process. A Case Based Discussion can easily turn into a DEBRIEF session as can using Pendleton’s rules for feedback. A learner can seek out a supervisor, more experienced colleague or a peer and discuss the event using the model outlined here. Alternatively the model can be applied to the event independently, using honest self disclosure to explore the issues and impact.

Conclusion

In a postgraduate medical training world where the pressure upon the trainers to teach has never been greater, but the time for teaching has never been more limited, the DEBRIEF model equips learners to structure and guide their own learning, utilising their supervisors, senior colleagues, peers and their own reflections to make sense of their daily experiences. Learning by pattern recognition is no longer tenable; smaller numbers of experiences carry greater pressure on trainees to learn and develop. DEBRIEF offers a comprehensive model for them to do this.


Bibliography

Boekaerts M (1993) Being concerned with well being and with learning Educational Psychologist 32(3) 137 - 151
Brooks JG & Brooks MG (1993) In search of understanding: the case for constructivist classrooms. Alexandria VA, Association for Supervision & Curriculum Development
Friere P (1970). Pedagogy of the oppressed. New York: Continuum
Goleman D (1995) Emotional Intelligence New York Bantam books
Gross M et al (2008) Emotions and feelings in learning process: Understanding emotional learning experiences of Postgraduate students ESREA Life History & Biography Network conference, Canterbury Christ Church university, UK http://tallinn.academia.edu/MarinGross/Papers/253692/Emotions_and_Feelings_In_Learning_Process_Understanding_Emotional_Learning_Experiences_of_Postgraduate_Students
ISCP 2010 https://www.iscp.ac.uk/home/principles_intro.aspx
Lave J, Wenger E (1991). Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Le Doux J (1997) The Emotional Brain: the mysterious underpinnings of emotional life New York Simon & Schuster
Love PG & Goodsell A (1996) Enhancing Student Learning: Intellectual, Social and Emotional Integration by Love. ASHE-ERIC Higher Education Report series 95-4, (Volume 24-4), http://www.ntlf.com/html/lib/bib/95-4dig.htm
Kolb D A (1984) Experiential Learning: Experience as the source of learning and development. Prentice-Hall.
RCP 2011 http://www.jrcptb.org.uk/assessment/Pages/Workplace-Based-Assessment.aspx
RCPath 2011 http://www.rcpath.org/resources/pdf/definitions_of_assessment_tools__ar.pdf
Pendleton’s Rules http://www.gp-training.net/training/educational_theory/feedback/pendleton.htm
Piaget, Jean. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child. Translated by Margeter Cook. New York: Ballantine.
Piaget, Jean. (1971). Psychology and Epistemology: Towards a Theory of Knowledge. Translated by Arnold Rosen. New York: The Viking Press.
Sylvester, R. (1994). How emotions affect learning. Educational Leadership, 52(2), 60-65.
Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind and society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wood, D. J., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 17(2), 89-100.